perm filename AKIDS[4,KMC] blob sn#053158 filedate 1973-07-11 generic text, type T, neo UTF8
00100		THE RATIONALE FOR COMPUTER-BASED TREATMENT OF
00200	        LANGUAGE DIFFICULTIES IN NONSPEAKING AUTISTIC
00300		             CHILDREN
00400	
00500		        KENNETH MARK COLBY
00600	
00700		No one person  could  conceive,  implement  and  utilize  the
00800	treatment method I shall discuss shortly. Hence let me express at the
00900	start my deep indebtedness to my co-workers at  Stanford  University-
01000	Horace  Enea,  David  Smith,  Malcolm Newey and Maxine Colby, each of
01100	whom has put years of effort into this project.
01200	
01300		We began about seven years ago with a notion and  two  facts.
01400	The notion was that a nonspeaking autistic child's primary difficulty
01500	lay in an inability to process symbols  (dissymbolia),language  being
01600	of   course   the  most  important  symbolic  system  used  in  human
01700	communication. The  first  fact,  oft  repeated  in  the  literature,
01800	indicated  that  prognosis  was  highly  correlated  with speech, the
01900	outlook for nonspeakers being poor.  The second fact was  the  common
02000	clinical  observation  that  these  children  played  for  hours with
02100	machines while remaining indifferent to interactions with people.
02200	
02300		There is now increasing evidence in the  research  literature
02400	supporting   this   notion   of   a   primary  difficulty  in  symbol
02500	processing.[Churchill,1972; Frith,1972; Hermelin  and  O'Connor,1970;
02600	Rutter,  Bartak,  and Newman,1971]. A dysphasic or aphasic child also
02700	has difficulty with  language  but  he  can  acquire  other  symbolic
02800	systems such a gesturing and drawing. He may even learn to read.  But
02900	the nonspeaking autisitc child has great difficulty with all symbolic
03000	processes, not just language, and hence can be called "dissymbolic".
03100	
03200		The  cause of this difficulty remains unknown.   Nowadays few
03300	experts  in  the  field  defend  a  psychogenic  etiology  since  the
03400	supporting evidence is weak and the disconfirming evidence is gaining
03500	in strength [Rutter,et al,1971]. Regardless of the original cause, if
03600	we  understand  the  crux  of the child's difficulty, we could try to
03700	devise a remedial treatment which takes advantage of  the  fact  that
03800	the  child  is fascinated by machines. Our beginning working idea was
03900	to create a machine a nonspeaking child could play  with in which the
04000	play process involves the child's use of language skills.
04100	
04200		The system we developed consists of a television-like  screen
04300	and  a  typewriter-like  keyboard.  Pressing the keys on the keyboard
04400	causes symbols to appear on the screen accompanied by sounds of human
04500	voices  and  other  noises  common in a child's life. It is much like
04600	having your own Sesame Street to play with. But rather than  being  a
04700	passive  recipient  of  what the television show provides, a child in
04800	our situation is an active initiating agent controlling the  machine.
04900	Instead  of  having  things done to him, things are done by him.  The
05000	merits of the machine are that it is  untiring,  predictable,  always
05100	saying  the  same  thing  the  same way, never angry, never bored and
05200	controllable- properties which are  notoriously  lacking  in  humans.
05300	This  audio-visual-  tactile  experience  is  provided  by a computer
05400	program running on a PDP-6/10 time-sheared computer in  the  Stanford
05500	Artificial  Intelligence  Project.  The program is divided into games
05600	intended to give the child various types of opportunities at  playing
05700	with  and  interacting with symbols. For example, in one game, when a
05800	child presses the key showing the letter  H,  an  H  appears  on  the
05900	screen  and  a  voice  says  "H".  In  another  game pressing the key
06000	labelled "H" produces a running horse on the  screen  accompanied  by
06100	the  sound  of horse's hoofs.  There exist over 1000 such experiences
06200	on  the  system.  The  games  are  organized  at  various  levels  of
06300	complexity  and  are  designed  to  show  a  child how English is put
06400	together from sounds and letters into words and expressions.  I shall
06500	not  go  into  the  details  of  the games here.   They are throughly
06600	described in the literature.[Colby and Smith,1971]. Instead  I  shall
06700	try  to  sketch  the  rationale  or  major principles underlying this
06800	approach.
06900	
07000		First,  consider  how  normal children acquire language. They
07100	are not taught formally as are adults  learning  a  second  language.
07200	Children  are simply exposed to members of a linguistic community and
07300	given  an  opportunity  to  explore  language   usage   in   everyday
07400	communication  between  themselves and other humans who encourage and
07500	correct.  From this exposure,  exploration  and  corrective  feedback
07600	they come to associate the sounds and meanings of words. They come to
07700	sense that certain sounds, human voice sounds, are not just sounds in
07800	themselves  like  the  wind  in  the  trees,  but that they are about
07900	something else, that is, they are  symbolic.   Between  the  ages  of
08000	roughly 2 months and 4 years normal children spend thousands of hours
08100	listening, practicing  and  playing  with  language.   Mastery  of  a
08200	language,  that  is getting most of it right, does not come until far
08300	into adolescence, if ever. A normal child can make an  interpretation
08400	of  expressions  he  has  never  heard before. A nonspeaking autistic
08500	child, being dissymbolic, does not show this course of development.
08600	
08700		The problem is not simply the development of language  skills
08800	but  also  the  acquisition of concepts necessary for a comprehensive
08900	enough model of how the world works, especially the human world.  The
09000	conceptual  or  cognitive  deficits  shown  by  nonspeaking  autistic
09100	children involve those concepts which are normally  acquired  through
09200	language  or  other  symbolizations.    Take  the abstract concept of
09300	`danger'.  To prevent a child  from  becoming  hurt,  a  parent  must
09400	identify  certain  concrete objects and situations as dangerous until
09500	the child grasps the abstract concept of danger.  All  this  is  done
09600	linguistically and through pointing. By way of language , objects and
09700	situations can be referred to and warned about even when they are not
09800	present,  and referred to without pointing when they are present.  An
09900	important socializing function of language is to mark off for a child
10000	what to pay attention to and what is to be done and not done. Many of
10100	the characteristics of autistic children, for example,  the  lack  of
10200	empathy,  can  be  viewed  as  a  consequence of an inability to form
10300	regular conceptual patterns about the  world  because  the  necessary
10400	concepts  acquired  through  language are missing.  As one perceptive
10500	mother said about her autistic child,` a screw is not loose , a screw
10600	is missing'.
10700	
10800		A poorly developed ability to  process  symbols  has  further
10900	consequences  besides  conceptual  deficits. Without language a human
11000	has no awareness of being aware. He lacks the ability to self-monitor
11100	and  to  self-control  by  talking to himself.  He cannot use symbols
11200	reflexively, that is, to to give himself orders  and  to  comment  on
11300	himself to  himself. Having language, a normal child comes to realize
11400	the self is really two which can talk to  one  another.  Finally,  to
11500	become  a  person  one must be recognized as a person by other people
11600	who treat you as if you had  self-awareness  and  self-control.  This
11700	recognition  is  virtually  impossible  for  a child without language
11800	skills and probably represents the greatest tragedy in his life.
11900	
12000		Thus  far  I  have  spoken  of  autistic  children  as if the
12100	nosological label stood for a single homogeneous group. Thirty  years
12200	ago  it  seemed  that  might  be the case. Now it appears as if there
12300	exist several autistic syndromes, none of which  should  be  confused
12400	with  childhood  schizophrenia  since  they  differ in onset, course,
12500	symptoms, family history and prognosis.     There  are  speaking  and
12600	nonspeaking  autistic children. Among the nonspeaking group there are
12700	those whose linguistic development is normal until  sometime  in  the
12800	second  year when they lose their language abilities. The other major
12900	group are those children who,  from  the  start,  have  trouble  with
13000	language,  understanding  little  and  saying  even less, perhaps one
13100	`mama' or `no' a year. In our experience the most  difficult  problem
13200	for  differential  diagnosis  lies  in deciding whether a nonspeaking
13300	child suffers  from  dysphasia  (developmental  aphasia),  autism  or
13400	perhaps  both.    Over time the correct diagnosis can be made when it
13500	becomes clear that the dysphasic child can mimic, draw  pictures  and
13600	signify greetings or farewells while the autistic child cannot.
13700	
13800	Taking a cue from the normal child who treats language as a toy,  our
13900	first  principle was that the treatment should provide an opportunity
14000	for exploratory play.  The treatment situation is not one  of  forced
14100	drill,  instruction or training but one of play with the keyboard and
14200	video display.  Operant conditioning methods reward  the  child  with
14300	candy  or  food  for  his  actions.    We do not, believing that food
14400	rewards inhibit exploratory curiosity, as has been shown  in  animals
14500	both  by  Harlow  and  by Nissen. From our own experience and that of
14600	workers in computer-aided instruction, food rewards have  been  found
14700	to  be  distracting  and  disrupting. Exploratory learning requires a
14800	situation which  invites  exploration,  time,  security  and  minimal
14900	interference by adults.
15000	
15100	In each of the sessions the child has a `sitter', an adult whose main
15200	task  is  to  sit  and not interfere. The sitter's task is difficult,
15300	especially if he has been trained to DO things.  We want to give  the
15400	child  an  opportunity  to  freely  self-select  those  symbols which
15500	interest him, rather than to have an adult instruct him or  quiz  him
15600	about  those  symbols which he `should' learn.  The sitter's behavior
15700	is crucial to this treatment method if the spirit of play rather than
15800	drill is to be provided.  Of course the sitter offers social approval
15900	and encouragement when it is fitting.
16000	
16100	An ideal treatment session results when the child is in a good  mood,
16200	is  interested  in working the keyboard display, enjoys imitating the
16300	sounds and is successful in getting the machine to do what he  wants.
16400	The principle of success is important here because, in our view, many
16500	nonspeakers have given up on language.  They  have  failed  over  and
16600	over  and hence withdraw from trying.  We do not let them fail.    As
16700	one normal child said about  the  experience  "it`s  fun,  you  can't
16800	lose". There are students of autism who consider nonspeaking autistic
16900	children to be innately withdrawn from people and hence not acquiring
17000	language.  We  feel  it  is  the  other way around; they have so much
17100	difficulty with language they withdraw from  people  who  unwittingly
17200	flood  and  overwhelm them with meaningless noises. No wonder they do
17300	not call, address, ask  or  answer  questions  of  these  giants  who
17400	immediately  spout  gibberish.   Autistic  children are not aloof and
17500	indifferent to all people -- only to those who  talk.  I  have  often
17600	wondered  if  is not eye-to-eye contact they avoid but eye-to-a-mouth
17700	which , as far as the child is concerned, simply jabbers. If you  say
17800	little  or  nothing  to a nonspeaking child on first meeting him, our
17900	experience has been that you will more than likely find him  in  your
18000	lap, as affectionate as any other child.
18100	
18200	There  exists  another  myth  in the literature which I would like to
18300	dispel. Autistic children are said to show pronominal reversal, using
18400	the  second  person  singular  pronoun  "you"  for  the  first person
18500	singular pronoun "I", and "I" for "you".  This is not so.  It is  the
18600	normal  child  who  reverses  or  converts  personal  pronouns.   The
18700	autistic child LACKS the rule for  correct  pronoun  assignments  and
18800	simply  echoes what he has heard.  Pronouns are examples of deixis in
18900	which the denotation of the  words  shift  depending  on  the  speech
19000	situation in contrast to, for example, proper names whose denotations
19100	remain constant. The assignment rule for the variables "I" and  "you"
19200	is  that  the speaker calls himself "I" and calls the listener "you".
19300	Young autistic children are ignorant  of  this  rule,  which  is  not
19400	actively taught by adults, but is somehow grasped by the normal child
19500	from the data of human dialogues. The autistic or dissymbolic  child,
19600	unable  to  process  language,  echoes  back, in an untransformed and
19700	uninterpreted way, what is said to him.   If you say to him  "Do  you
19800	want your jacket" he will echo it, failing to transform the pronouns.
19900	Everybody calls him "you" so he believes "you" must  be  one  of  his
20000	names.    Due  to  the  limitations of short-term memory, an autistic
20100	child may echo only the terminal fragment of a long  expression.   In
20200	English  "I"  regularly  appears at the beginning of an expression as
20300	the subject.  If  you  place  the  pronoun  "I"  at  the  end  of  an
20400	expression,  an  autistic  child can echo it as easily as "you". Some
20500	people believe that the pronoun "I" is an index of self-identity  and
20600	that  autistic  children  lack a sense of self. From our experience I
20700	would say that  autistic  children  make  a  very  clear  distinction
20800	between  self  and  non-self.  They  are  in fact hyperautonomous, as
20900	anyone will find out when he tries to get the child to  do  something
21000	he  does  not want to do.  Autistic children do not use "I" and "you"
21100	correctly because they are incognizant of a  symbolic  transformation
21200	rule  necessary  for  the  correct assignment and conversion of input
21300	pronouns.  It is worth noting that even in normal children ,  "I"  is
21400	never triggered by "you" but only by "me". [Fay,1969].
21500	
21600	Returning from this digression on deixis, let me say something  about
21700	our  successes  and  our  failures,  especially  the  latter.   Every
21800	treatment method reports its  dramatic  successes  with  one  or  two
21900	cases.  What  we  need  are  long  case  series before we can compare
22000	methods and decide which is more effective  relative  to  the  effort
22100	required.   Thus  far  our  series  of  nonspeaking autistic children
22200	numbers 17 with 13 of the children improved.  By improvement we mean
22300	only  that  the  child  begins  voluntarily  to use speech for social
22400	communication.  We do not claim the method results in normal language
22500	ability  with  full  comprehension, and correct pronunciation, syntax
22600	and grammar.   Our  aim  is  to  rekindle  the  child's  interest  in
22700	attempting  speech,  to  get  him  to  try  again, to get him off the
22800	ground, to stimulate or catalyze a damaged or slow-developing natural
22900	process   of   language   acquisition.    
23000		We have tried the method  with  other  types  of  nonspeaking
23100	children.   We  failed  with  two  schizophrenic children.  One brain
23200	damaged child improved and one did not.  We failed completely with  3
23300	children  suffering  from  expressive  aphasia.  By failure we mean a
23400	child leaves us as he came, perhaps with some language  understanding
23500	but producing no useful speech.
23600	
23700	In  studying  our failures among the autistic group, we have tried to
23800	find some common denominator.   We have found only two  in  our  case
23900	series  of  17  nonspeaking  dissymbolic  children.  First, all these
24000	failure cases are children who showed no interest in playing with the
24100	machine.   Regardless  of  our coaxing and persuading they would have
24200	nothing to do with it.   Finally, becoming  desperate,  we  would  be
24300	forced  to violate one of our basic principles of allowing free play.
24400	We would spend weeks and months holding the child  at  the  keyboard,
24500	pushing  the  keys  for  him,  trying  to overcome his resistance and
24600	negativism.   But to no avail. Somehow we must be more  ingenious  in
24700	capturing  these  children's interest.  We have thought of ways to do
24800	this and are currently trying to implement them.
24900	
25000	Second, in cases where the child shows normal linguistic  development
25100	until 16 or 22 months and then suddenly stops talking, we have failed
25200	absolutely.   This history is suggestive of course of  some  type  of
25300	cerebral  insult  such  as a virus infection or auto-immune reaction,
25400	but these hypotheses remain unconfirmed.
25500	
25600	Based on our experience we believe there is  something  powerful  but
25700	not  well-understood  about  the  method.    As  I  described  it, he
25800	treatment is a complex variable, a shotgun prescription,  the  active
25900	ingredients  of which are unclear. We need other workers in the field
26000	to adopt the method, improve it and hopefully help us  find  stronger
26100	catalysts for language acquisition.
26200	
26300	Why haven't  other  workers  tried  this  zero-risk  and  potentially
26400	promising  method?  Aside from the inertia which all new methods must
26500	face, there are three reasons.   One is that  everyone  has  his  own
26600	preferred  method  which  he  believes  in and may wish to improve. A
26700	second reason involves  people's  beliefs  and  misconceptions  about
26800	computers. I have come across people with the following fantasy about
26900	our situation -- that we have a  small,  cowering,  bewildered  child
27000	sitting  in  front  of  a  towering  bank of flashing lights {the New
27100	Yorker-cartoon concept of a computer} while cold scientists in  white
27200	coats  observe  him  thru  one-way mirrors.    They say "the child is
27300	already too interested in machines, he needs relations with people --
27400	you are making him into even more of a robot".   I hope from my brief
27500	description you can see this fantasy is not the case.  It  may  sound
27600	paradoxical,  but  some  nonspeaking  children can become more human,
27700	i.e. become language users, by way of a machine which talks and which
27800	they  find  more  acceptable  on their own terms than they do talking
27900	humans.
28000	
28100	A more realistic objection to computers is their cost.  Few people in
28200	the  world  have  access to million-dollar computers for this sort of
28300	work.  But a large computer  is  not  necessary  to  carry  out  this
28400	method.   Mini-computers  costing  only  a  few  thousand dollars are
28500	adequate.  It may be that we do not need a computer at all if someone
28600	found  a  way to rapidly random-accessg both sounds and pictures.  We
28700	are investigating this possibility. Once we can get the  misconceived
28800	Satanic image of the computer out of the picture , I hope others will
28900	join us  in  discovering  more  about  this  technique  for  treating
29000	nonspeaking dissymbolic children.
29100	
29200			REFERENCES
29300	
29400	
29500	[1] Churchill,D.W.,1972. The relation of infantile autism and early
29600	       childhood schizophrenia to developmental language disorders
29700	       of childhood. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia,
29800	       2,182-197.
29900	[2] Colby, K.M. and Smith, D.C., 1971. Computers in the treatment of
30000	       nonspeaking autistic children. In Current Psychiatric Therapies,
30100	       Masserman, J.H.(Ed.), Grune and Stratton, New York.
30200	[3] Fay, W.H., 1969. On normal and autistic pronouns. Journal of Speech
30300	       and Hearing,36,242-249.
30400	
30500	
30600	[4] Frith, U., 1972. Cognitive mechanisms in autism: experiments with
30700	        color and tone sequence production. Journal of Autism and
30800	        Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 160-173.
30900	[5] Harlow, H.(1953). Motivation as a factor in the acquisition of
31000		new responses. Current theory and research in motivation.
31100		University of Nebraska Press.
31200	[6] Hermelin, B. and O'Connor, N., 1970. Psychological Experiments
31300	        With Autistic Children, Pergamon Press, London.
31400	[7] Nissen,H.W. (1954). The nature of the drive as innate determinant
31500		of behavior organization. Nebraska Symposium On
31600		Motivation,9,308.
31700	[8] Rutter, M., Bartak, L. and Newman, S., 1971. Autism- a central
31800	        disorder of cognition and language? In Infantile Autism,
31900	        Rutter, M.(Ed.), Churchill Livingstone, London.
32000	
32100	
32200	
32300			ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
32400	
32500	
32600	This research is supported by Grant PHS MH 06645-12 from the National
32700	Institute  of Mental Health and by (in part) Reaearch Scientist Award
32800	(No. 1-K05-K-14,333) from the National Institute of Mental Health.